Sunday
Scientists Drug - Test Whole Cities
http://www.nytimes.com/aponline/us/AP-Citywide-Drug-Test.html?_r=1&oref=slogin
August 21, 2007
Scientists Drug - Test Whole Cities
By THE ASSOCIATED PRESS
Filed at 9:08 p.m. ET
WASHINGTON (AP) -- Researchers have figured out how to give an entire
community a drug test using just a teaspoon of wastewater from a city's
sewer plant.
The test wouldn't be used to finger any single person as a drug user.
But it would help federal law enforcement and other agencies track the
spread of dangerous drugs, like methamphetamines, across the country.
Oregon State University scientists tested 10 unnamed American cities for
remnants of drugs, both legal and illegal, from wastewater streams. They
were able to show that they could get a good snapshot of what people are
taking.
''It's a community urinalysis,'' said Caleb Banta-Green, a University of
Washington drug abuse researcher who was part of the Oregon State team.
The scientists presented their results Tuesday at a meeting of the
American Chemical Society in Boston.
Two federal agencies have taken samples from U.S. waterways to see if
drug testing a whole city is doable, but they haven't gotten as far as
the Oregon researchers.
One of the early results of the new study showed big differences in
methamphetamine use city to city. One urban area with a gambling
industry had meth levels more than five times higher than other cities.
Yet methamphetamine levels were virtually nonexistent in some smaller
Midwestern locales, said Jennifer Field, the lead researcher and a
professor of environmental toxicology at Oregon State.
The ingredient Americans consume and excrete the most was caffeine,
Field said.
Cities in the experiment ranged from 17,000 to 600,000 in population,
but Field declined to identify them, saying that could harm her
relationship with the sewage plant operators.
She plans to start a survey for drugs in the wastewater of at least 40
Oregon communities.
The science behind the testing is simple. Nearly every drug -- legal and
illicit -- that people take leaves the body. That waste goes into
toilets and then into wastewater treatment plants.
''Wastewater facilities are wonderful places to understand what humans
consume and excrete,'' Field said.
In the study presented Tuesday, one teaspoon of untreated sewage water
from each of the cities was tested for 15 different drugs. Field said
researchers can't calculate how many people in a town are using drugs.
She said that one fairly affluent community scored low for illicit drugs
except for cocaine. Cocaine and ecstasy tended to peak on weekends and
drop on weekdays, she said, while methamphetamine and prescription drugs
were steady throughout the week.
Field said her study suggests that a key tool currently used by drug
abuse researchers -- self-reported drug questionnaires -- underestimates
drug use.
''We have so few indicators of current use,'' said Jane Maxwell of the
Addiction Research Institute at the University of Texas, who wasn't part
of the study. ''This could be a very interesting new indicator.''
David Murray, chief scientist for U.S. Office of National Drug Control
Policy, said the idea interests his agency.
Murray said the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency is testing federal
wastewater samples just to see if that's a good method for monitoring
drug use. But he didn't know how many tests were conducted or where.
The EPA will ''flush out the details'' on testing, Benjamin Grumbles
joked. The EPA assistant administrator said the agency is already
looking at the problem of potential harm to rivers and lakes from legal
pharmaceuticals.
The idea of testing on a citywide basis for drugs makes sense, as long
as it doesn't violate people's privacy, said Tom Angell of the Students
for Sensible Drug Policy, a Washington-based group that wants looser
drug laws.
''This seems to be less offensive than individualized testing,'' he
said.
Dutch Consider Magic Mushroom Ban-Aug. 08, 2007
Dutch Consider Magic Mushroom Ban
http://www.time.com/time/printout/0,8816,1650873,00.html
By Joost van Egmond/Amsterdam
Time Magazine
When Amsterdam police found a disoriented French tourist in his van last
month with his slain dog beside him, he told them he had wanted to free
the animal's mind. He also said he had ingested magic mushrooms, which
contain the hallucinogen psilocybin. The incident played into a running
debate over whether the Netherlands' famously liberal drug laws are too
lax with psychedelic mushrooms. Also in July, a Danish tourist raced his
car through a campsite, and a 19-year old man from Iceland jumped out of
a window; both had taken magic mushrooms, known in Dutch as "paddos," as
had a French teenager who jumped off a bridge to her death in March.
Since then, most parties in the Dutch parliament have been calling for a
clampdown on magic mushrooms. In dried form, the fungi are already
prohibited, but fresh mushrooms can still be legally sold in the
Netherlands. The country's public health minister, Ab Klink, has so far
steered clear of banning psilocybin mushrooms altogether, in part
because his ministry considers it legally problematic to ban a product
that grows naturally. But in May he commissioned fresh research into the
risks of "paddo" use, and has said he would consider the results, due
next month, in deciding how to act.
This being the Netherlands, critics say even that measured reaction is
too precipitous. They argue that while "paddo" use may have been
involved in serious incidents, it's too easy to single out the drug as
the cause of them. Municipal heath services determined that the man who
killed his dog had a psychosis unrelated to the drug, and the Danish
racer consumed alcohol and smoked marijuana before taking his "paddo."
Amsterdam municipal heath services report that the number of
mushroom-related incidents, while rising, is still dwarfed by problems
caused by alcohol. Advocates of a ban counter that the easy availability
of magic mushrooms amounts to an invitation to further tragedies.
There is general agreement, however, that foreigners seem to have more
trouble with 'shrooms that the Dutch themselves do. In Amsterdam, some
90 percent of ambulance dispatches related to magic mushroom use this
year were for foreign visitors, especially from Britain, trailed at a
distance by Italy, the U.S. and France. "Most problems are caused by
foreigners who come here on cheap flights to take as many drugs as they
can find," says Guy Boels, chairman of VLOS, an association of Dutch
magic mushrooms retailers. "They hardly sleep, they drink alcohol and
smoke pot as much as they can and then take a paddo on top of that."
Boels says the risks of reckless behavior are quite small as long as
paddos are not mixed with alcohol or drugs. Still, VLOS supports a
proposed regulation to ban the sale of the mushrooms to minors and calls
for a registration system to identify "weekend tourists." For now, that
watchful but tolerant approach is getting the endorsement of Dutch
public heath experts. Unless the new research commissioned by the
minister arrives at new insights, the government appears more likely to
play the regulation card than to support a total ban on magic mushrooms.
Human-subjects research: Trial and error
Human-subjects research: Trial and error
Heidi Ledford1
The ethics committees that oversee research done in humans have beenattacked from all sides. Heidi Ledford recounts the struggle to come upwith alternatives.Fourteen years of treating people with tuberculosis has taught physicianWilliam Burman what to expect when a patient walks through his door.Tuberculosis is not typically a disease of the well-heeled. Manypatients in the United States are foreign born. English is their secondlanguage. Fewer than half have completed a high-school education, andmany have spent time in jails or homeless shelters.So when Burman, of the University of Colorado in Denver, joined in twostudies run by the Tuberculosis Trials Consortium, he knew that theconsent forms needed to cater to people with an eighth-grade readinglevel (comprehensible to an educated 13-year-old). The trials involvedmultiple institutions, and the forms were sent to 39 institutionalreview boards (IRBs) - committees designed to determine whether aproposed experiment is ethically sound. The final approvals came in 346days later, but what the IRBs sent back, Burman found disturbing."The consent forms were longer. The language was more complex," Burmansays. "And errors were inserted at a surprising frequency." In one case,a potential negative side effect of the treatment had been accidentallyedited out. Burman responded to the problem as any researcher would: hestudied it. He had an independent panel review the changes. Thereviewers found that 85% of the changes did not affect the meaning ofthe consent forms, but that the average reading level had jumped fromthat of an eighth grader to that of a twelfth grader (around 17 yearsold)1. His results confirmed something he'd suspected for some time. "Istarted to think about what was happening and it just seemed like thesystem was flawed." It was time to change the system.Burman is not alone. In the 40 years since their birth (see 'Time forethics'), IRBs, also known as research ethics committees, have facedcriticism from all sides. They're too slow, or too hasty,overprotective, or they flout basic safety. They're bureaucratic,wasteful and unavoidable. So, what are researchers to do? The will forchange exists, says Sarah Greene, a researcher at the Group HealthCenter for Health Studies in Seattle, Washington. But recent attempts tofix the system have struggled to gain a foothold.Figure 1: Time for ethicsHigh resolution image and legend (221K)Obstacle courseIn many countries, a complex network of local ethics committees handlesthe approval of research on humans. This focus on local resources allowscommittees to account for specific laws or cultural concerns in aparticular region. But it leads to problems in multicentre trials, suchas Burman's, which are becoming more frequent. When IRBs were firstfounded, multicentre trials were almost unheard of. A 1998 report2 fromthe inspector-general of the US Department of Health and Human Servicesin Washington DC stated that a rise in the number of multicentre studieswas throwing the system into crisis. And a recent analysis3 showed thatfive of 20 trials seeking IRB approval reported significant delays as aresult of IRB negotiations. Seventeen noted inconsistencies both inIRBs' review process and in their recommendations. In one case,negotiations between 65 IRBs delayed the study by a year.More frighteningly, the cumbersome system could even endanger the healthof the studies' participants. The higher the hurdles - and the moreunfair they seem - the less inclined researchers will be to jump them."It's slow and frustrating to researchers," says Ezekiel Emanuel, chairof the US Department of Bioethics at the National Institutes of Healthin Bethesda, Maryland. Researchers have reported that they are morelikely to violate the regulations set by ethics committee if they feelthat they or their application have been mishandled4.And even though IRBs are made up mostly of volunteers, they areexpensive to run. In 2002, the median cost of running an IRB, takinginto account the time spent by IRB members, was $742,000; the maximumwas over $4 million5. So, for every protocol they assess, they charge afee to cover support staff, facilities, and outside consulting. Thesefees are typically pulled from grants as part of the institutionaloverhead, or as direct charges to commercial sponsors, and they averagejust over US$1000 (ref. 5).A proposed solution to the copious problems with IRBs is outsourcing,especially for multicentre studies, to some form of centralized review.That movement has met with resistance from those who say that localreview provides valuable local context. But Burman counters that localcontext had little bearing on the changes in his consent forms. In histrials, only 1.5% of the tweaks to the consent forms were made toaccount for local context1.Risky processBut that's not enough to rule out the importance of local review arguesDavid Wynes, vice-president for research administration at EmoryUniversity in Atlanta, Georgia. "I agree that the vast majority ofchanges are editorial, but I think there's a value in an institutionhaving a process for identifying when local context is an issue," hesays. "You might have to review a hundred protocols before you can seethe value of local context. Is it OK if only 1% of the time you putsubjects at risk?"Is it OK if only 1% of the time you put subjects at risk?David WynesBurman argues that expertise with a specific patient group or diseaseshould trump local context from detached review boards. "The local IRBsdon't know the patients I take care of, because if they did, the lastthing they would do is increase the length of the consent form and makethe language more complex," he says. Instead, Burman and his colleagueshave worked to create a designated panel at the Centers for DiseaseControl and Prevention (CDC) in Atlanta, Georgia, which keeps track ofdisease epidemiology in the United States, to review all tuberculosisstudies.Top trumpsBut a centralized system will work only if the local boards are notallowed to overrule the decision of the central board, says Emanuel.It's a lesson, he adds, that the United Kingdom has had to learn thehard way.In 1997, the United Kingdom created a system of regional review boardsin which trials needed approval from just one board to proceed. In 2000,the system was brought under the auspices of the Central Office ofResearch Ethics Committees (COREC), based in London. The trouble was,local ethics committees refused to surrender control, and instead ofexpediting review, COREC had created a new layer of bureaucracy."Researchers were very upset with the way things were going," says EmmaCave, a lecturer at the Leeds School of Law, UK. "They thought theregulations were making the United Kingdom a bad place for research." InApril, Britain dissolved COREC in favour of the new National ResearchEthics Service, and changed the regulations to restrict the ability oflocal ethics committees to change the protocol approved by the nationaloffice.In 2001, the US National Cancer Institute (NCI) in Bethesda, Maryland,launched a similar experiment - a central review board to review allNCI-funded research on humans. Local review boards retained the power todo a full review, but could opt instead for an expedited review in whichthey merely adjust for local context. (The NCI formed a similar reviewboard for paediatric studies in 2004.)The project immediately ran into trouble. The central board spent toomuch effort on scientifically reviewing proposals that had already beenreviewed by the granting arm of NCI, says Richard Schilsky, chairman ofCancer and Leukemia Group B, an NCI-sponsored cancer clinical trialsgroup. And at first, few local IRBs were willing to cede control to thecentral review board. "The concept is good," says Schilsky, "but thedevil has been in the details of the implementation."Since 2001, the number of participating institutions has climbed to 300.More than half of those have accepted the reviews of NCI's centralboard; the remainder are still developing ways to incorporate thecentral boards review into their own review process.. But Schilsky saysthat only about 20% of the institutions involved in his clinical-trialsgroup - the largest in the United States - have signed on. The resultwas similar to what happened in Britain, adding to the bureaucracy.Schilsky estimates that the system has added two to three months to thetime it takes to activate a new study.Lainie Ross, a paediatrician and member of the IRB at the University ofChicago Medical School, says that she is opposed to surrendering localcontrol. "A national IRB could fail to recognize different needs ofdifferent communities. I'm not just going to accept someone else's wordfor it."A national IRB could fail to recognize different needs of differentcommunities.Lainie RossWithout fail, IRB members interviewed by Nature who were opposed toceding control to a centralized board cited concerns about patientsafety as their main reason. But Emanuel, who has also served on an IRB,says that there's another cause for concern. "There are no good datasuggesting that there are local factors that are ethically relevant,"says Emanuel. "It's really liability that's driving this."Vulnerable populationsLiability is a thorny issue for local IRBs contemplating handing overcontrol to NCI's central IRB, says Wynes. If a participant in a clinicaltrial felt that he or she were unjustly harmed during the course of theresearch, they could not hold the NCI legally responsible because it isa branch of the federal government. That leaves the local IRB legallyvulnerable, says Clint Hermes, general council at St. Jude Children'sResearch Hospital in Memphis, Tennessee.It is rare, but IRBs and even individual IRB members have been sued inthe past. Bioethicist Arthur Caplan at the University of Pennsylvania inPhiladelphia says that he has served on two IRBs that were sued butstill thinks it's important to have a mechanism in place to holdnegligent IRBs accountable.D. PARKINSMeanwhile, a profitable industry in private, commercial IRBs has sprungup. Although commercial IRBs can provide a sense of security by assuminglegal liability, the institution doing the experiment will bear ultimateresponsibility. But partial indemnity seems sufficient to comfort manyresearchers: commercial IRBs serve hundreds of companies, hospitals andresearch institutions. In 2005, the consulting firm Deloitte namedChesapeake Research Review of Columbia, Maryland - a commercial IRB andconsulting service - as one of the fastest-growing technology companiesin North America. The firm increased its revenues by 244% in five years,to reach nearly $5.5 million in 2004.Proponents of commercial IRBs say that larger companies have goodreputations for speedy turnaround and thorough reviews. Several,including the two largest players, Chesapeake Research Review andWestern IRB in Olympia, Washington, have been officially accredited bythe Association for the Accreditation of Human Research ProtectionPrograms in Washington DC. Such societies provide a stamp of approvalfor IRBs, providing oversight and standardization to the field.Financial gainStill, others worry about the potential conflict of interest inherent tocommercial IRBs, who could benefit financially from pleasing theircustomers and passing protocols with minimal fuss. "I'm a littlecautious about this drive towards commercial IRBs," says Richard Bianco,associate vice-president for regulatory affairs at the University ofMinnesota in Minneapolis, and a 15-year IRB veteran. Nevertheless,Bianco and other critics acknowledge that local IRBs also have aconflict of interest - clinical trials can bring in serious cash andprestige to the institutions they serve, and IRB members that are alsoscientists at the institution may feel pressured to approve a trial."I'm somewhat surprised that no one has ever pushed to reform theprivate side of IRBs," says Caplan. "It's growing like crazy. Industryhires them because they're fast and efficient. It doesn't mean thatthey're right."Industry hires private IRBs because they're fast and efficient. Itdoesn't mean that they're right.Arthur CaplanBianco also says that his colleagues have been under "intense pressure"by industry collaborators to relinquish control to commercial IRBs. Sometrial sponsors, he says, even issue ultimatums: use the commercial IRBthat we recommend or don't participate in the trial. "That waspressure," says Bianco. "But I've been around a long time. You come toknow what to ignore." None of those threats ever came to fruition, hesays.ADVERTISEMENTFor others, initial scepticism of commercial IRBs has given way toacceptance. "When I first came across independent IRBs, I questionedthem, too," says Wynes. "But I've taken the time to get to know how theyoperate, and my comfort level has changed." In November 2005, whileWynes was still at the University of Iowa in Iowa City, he helped theuniversity to switch to outsourcing industry-sponsored trials to WesternIRB. Prices vary, but outsourcing to industry can cost twice as much asprocessing the application in-house, although commercial IRBs typicallyboast a quicker turnaround time. Whereas many commercial IRBs aim toreview applications within a week of their receipt, non-commercial IRBsmay meet only once a month.Some see commercial IRBs as a stop-gap measure in lieu of realregulatory change. But despite the roadblocks, substantial change isinevitable, says Emanuel. The lingering problem, he adds, is that itwill probably take a new scandal to push reform to the top of theagenda. "I think we're just one accident away, but it will still takethe accident," he says. "In my opinion, that's the sad fact."See Editorial, page 511.Top of pageReferencesBurman, W. et al. Control. Clin. Trials 24, 245-255 (2003).Office of the Inspector General, Department of Health and HumanServices. Institutional review boards: a time for reform. (US GovernmentPrinting Office, Washington, DC, 1998).Greene, S. M. & Geiger, A. M. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 59, 784-790 (2006).Keith-Spiegel, P. & Koocher, G. P. Ethics Behav. 15, 339-349 (2005).Sugarman, J. et al. N. Engl. J. Med. 352, 1825-1827 (2005).
Heed the warning on the vial: "Do not snort Blow."
Remember that "Cocaine" energy drink that got pulled from the market?Now there's "Blow".Website and news story follow below.-------
http://www.iloveblow.com/-------
http://www.philly.com/philly/health_and_science/20070709_Its_Blow__an_energy_drink_.html
Blow is a white energy powder, with plenty of caffeine, and is designed to mix in drinks, particularly alcoholic.Erika GebelPhiladelphia InquirerIt's a white powder, it'll keep you wired all night, and it's called Blow.But it's not cocaine. It's a crystalline energy drink, a sweet mix like Kool-Aid, aimed at the bar scene."Our product is not designed to be an illicit-drug alternative," says Logan Gola, the brains behind Blow. Still, it arrived at The Inquirer in a faux dusty box. Inside were vials of Blow, a toy credit card, and a mirror. (But no dollar bill.)The new mix is being peddled to a market that is hooked: Energy-drink sales increased by 50 percent between 2005 and 2006, according to the Beverage Marketing Corp.Blow, like most energy drinks, includes a cavalcade of impressive-sounding ingredients - taurine, B vitamins, inositol, L-carnitine - but the tried-and-true upper is the ever-present caffeine. Perhaps too much."There have been alarming rates of all these health problems associated with high caffeine," said Lisa Hark, director of the Nutrition Education and Prevention Program at the University of Pennsylvania. To find out how much is too much, go to www.energyfiend.com's death-by-caffeine calculator.Then there's the booze issue. Red Bull, the flagship energy drink, is often mixed with vodka or taken with a shot of the liqueur Jägermeister, the widely guzzled "Jäger Bomb." In the April 2006 issue of Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, researchers reported that drinking Red Bull made subjects feel less drunk, but not act less drunk.The impact of a drink mix like Blow forges new territory. It cuts out the liquid middleman and can be dissolved directly into any cocktail. "It's being sold in liquor stores," said Lauren Seal, Blow promoter. "People have put it in anything: shots, vodka-based cocktails, gin."But heed the warning on the vial: "Do not snort Blow."
Thursday
Monday
LA TIMES: This is your brain on love - When you're attracted to someone, is your gray matter talking sense -- or just hooked?
This is your brain on loveWhen you're attracted to someone, is your gray matter talking sense -- or just hooked? Scientists take a rational look.
By Susan BrinkLos Angeles Times Staff Writer
July 30, 2007Her front brain is telling her he's trouble. Look at the facts, it says. He's never made a commitment, he drinks too much, he can't hold down a job.But her middle brain won't listen. Man, it swoons, he looks great in those jeans, his black hair curls onto his forehead so adorably, and when he drags on a cigarette, he's so bad he's good.His front brain is lecturing, too: She's flirting with every guy in the place, and she can drink even you under the table, it says. His mid-brain is unresponsive, distracted by her legs, her blouse and her come-hither stare."What could you be thinking?" their front brains demand.Their middle brains, each on a quest for reward, pay no heed.Alas, when it comes to choosing mates, smart neurons can make dumb choices. Sure, if the brain's owner is in her 40s and has been around the block a few times, she might grab her bag and scram. If the guy has reached seasoned middle age, he might think twice about that cleavage-baring temptress. Wisdom -- at least a little -- does come with experience.But if the objects of desire are in their 20s, all bets are off. A lot will depend on the influence of Mom and Dad's marriage, the gossip and urgings of friends, and whether life experience has convinced these two brains that what they're looking at is attractive. She just might sidle over to Mr. Wrong and bat her eyes. And he could well give in to temptation.And so the dance of attraction, infatuation and ultimately love begins.It's a dance that holds many mysteries, to psychologists as well as to the willing participants. Science is just beginning to parse the inner workings of the brain in love, examining the blissful or ruinous fall from a medley of perspectives: neural systems, chemical messengers and the biology of reward.It was only in 2000 that two London scientists selected 70 people, all in the early sizzle of love, and rolled them into the giant cylinder of a functional magnetic resonance imaging scanner, or fMRI. The images they got are thought to be science's first pictures of the brain in love.The pictures were a revelation, and others have followed, showing that romantic love is a lot like addiction to alcohol or drugs. The brain is playing a trick, necessary for evolution, by associating something that just happened with pleasure and attributing the feeling to that magnificent specimen right before your eyes.All animals mate: The most primitive system in the brain, one that even reptiles have, knows it needs to reproduce. Turtles do it but then lay their eggs in the sand and head back to sea, never seeing their mate again.Human brains are considerably more complicated, with additional neural systems that seek romance, others that want comfort and companionship, and others that are just out for a roll in the hay.Yet the chemistry between two people isn't just a matter of molecules careening around the brain, dictating feelings like some game of neuro-billiards. Attraction also involves personal history. "Our parents have an effect on us," says Helen Fisher, evolutionary anthropologist at Rutgers University who studies human attraction. "So does the school system, television, timing, mystery."Every book ever read, and every movie ever wept through, starts charting a course toward the chosen one.The love dance"Love," that one small word, stands for a hodgepodge of feelings and drives: lust, romance, passion, attachment, commitment and contentment. Studying this brew is made harder because the pathways aren't totally distinct. Lust and romance, for example, have some overlapping biology, even though they are not the same thing.Similarly, the dance that leads, if we're lucky, to a stable commitment moves through several key steps.First comes initial attraction, the spark. If someone's going to pick one person out of the billions of opposite-sex humans out there, it's this step that starts things rolling.Next comes the wild, dizzying infatuation of romance -- a unique magic between two people who can't stop thinking about each other. The brain uses its chemical arsenal to focus our attention on one person, forsaking all others."Everyone knows what that feels like. This is one of the great mysteries. It's the love potion No. 9, the click factor, interpersonal chemistry," says Gian Gonzaga, senior research scientist at eHarmony Labs.The passion lasts at least for a few months, two to four years tops, says relationship researcher Arthur Aron, psychologist at the State University of New York at Stony Brook.As it fades, something more stable takes over: the steady pair-bonding of what's called companionate love. It's a heartier variety, characterized by tenderness, affection and stability over the long haul. Far less is known about the brains of people celebrating their silver anniversaries or more, but researchers are beginning to recruit such couples to find out.When Kelly and Robert Iblings of Calabasas had their first face-to-face meeting after a month of corresponding online, all signs of a spark were there. Kelly, 30, recalls thinking "Wow!" Robert, 33, thought Kelly was beautiful. "I love his height," Kelly says of Robert's 6-foot-4 frame. "And those eyes. He's quite handsome. I mean, look at him. He's cute. He's hot.""She's very cute," Robert says. "And I like the way she laughs."Their brains' signals were in sync, and it was good.It probably didn't hurt that they were a little bit nervous about meeting each other.For years, scientists have known that attraction is more likely to happen when people are aroused, be it through laughter, anxiety or fear. Aron tested that theory in 1974 on the gorgeous but spine-chilling heights of the Capilano Canyon Suspension Bridge in Vancouver, British Columbia -- a 5-foot wide, 450-foot, wobbly, swaying length of wooden slats and wire cable suspended 230 feet above rocks and shallow rapids.His research team waited as unsuspecting men, between ages 18 and 35 and unaccompanied by women, crossed over. About halfway across the bridge, each man ran into an attractive young woman claiming to be doing research on beautiful places. She asked him a few questions and gave him her phone number in case he had follow-up questions.The experiment was repeated upriver on a bridge that was wide and sturdy and only 10 feet above a small rivulet. The same attractive coed met the men, brandishing the same questionnaire.The result? Men crossing the scary bridge rated the woman on the Capilano bridge more attractive. And about half the men who met her called her afterward. Only two of 16 men on the stable bridge called.Fear got their attention and aroused emotional centers in the brain. "People are more likely to feel aroused in a scary setting," Aron says. "It's pretty simple. You're feeling physiologically aroused, and it's ambiguous why. Then you see an attractive person, and you think, 'Oh, that's why.' "In a laboratory, Aron tested his arousal theory further by having people run in place for 10 minutes, and compared them with people who didn't run. Those who had exercised were more attracted to good-looking people in photographs than those who had been sedentary.Any kind of physiological arousal would probably do the trick, Aron concludes from his studies. Couples who ride roller coasters, laugh at a really funny comedian or escape a burning building together get an emotional jolt and could attribute the feeling to the attractiveness of the other.The forces of attraction are in many ways mysterious, but scientists know certain things. Studies have shown that women prefer men with symmetrical faces and that men like a certain waist-to-hip ratio in their mates. One study even found that women, when they sniffed men's T-shirts, were attracted to certain kinds of body odors.That initial spark can flash and fade. Or it can become a flame and then a fire, a rush of exhilaration, yearning, hunger and sense of complete union that scientists know as passionate love.Key to this state of seeing a person as a soul mate instead of a one-night stand is the limbic system, nestled deep within the brain between the neocortex (the region responsible for reason and intellect) and the reptilian brain (responsible for primitive instincts). Altered levels of dopamine, norepinephrine and serotonin -- neurotransmitters also associated with arousal -- wield their influence.But passionate love is something far stronger than that first sizzle of chemistry. "It's a drive to win life's greatest prize, the right mating partner," Fisher says. It is also, she adds, an addiction.People in the early throes of passionate love, she says, can think of little else. They describe sleeplessness, loss of appetite, feelings of euphoria, and they're willing to take exceptional risks for the loved one.Brain areas governing reward, craving, obsession, recklessness and habit all play their part in the trickery.In an experiment published as a chapter in a 2006 book, "Evolutionary Cognitive Neuroscience," Fisher found 17 people who were in relationships for an average of seven months. She knew they were in love from their answers to what researchers call the Passionate Love Scale. They all said they'd feel deep despair if their lover left, and they yearned to know all there was to know about the loved one.She put these lovesick, enraptured people in an fMRI to see what areas of their brains got active when they saw a photograph of their beloved ones."We found some remarkable things," she said. "We saw activity in the ventral tegmental area and other regions of the brain's reward system associated with motivation, elation and focused attention." It's the same part of the brain that presumably is active when a smoker reaches for a cigarette or when gamblers think they're going to win the lottery. No wonder it's as hard to say no to the feeling of romantic arousal as it would be to say no to a windfall in the millions. The brain has seen what it wants, and it's going to get it."At that point, you really wouldn't notice if he had three heads," Fisher says. "Or you'd notice, but you'd choose to overlook it."Other studies also suggest that the brain in the first throes of love is much like a brain on drugs.Lucy Brown, professor of neuroscience at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine, has also taken fMRI images of people in the early days of a new love. In a study reported in the July 2005, Journal of Neurophysiology, she too found key activity in the ventral tegmental area. "That's the area that's also active when a cocaine addict gets an IV injection of cocaine," Brown says. "It's not a craving. It's a high."You see someone, you click, and you're euphoric. And in response, your ventral tegmental area uses chemical messengers such as dopamine, serotonin and oxytocin to send signals racing to a part of the brain called the nucleus accumbens with the good news, telling it to start craving."The other person becomes a goal in your life," Brown says. He or she becomes a goal you might die without and would pack up and move across the country for. That one person begins to stand out as the one and only.Biologically, the cravings and pleasure unleashed are as strong as any drug. Surely such a goal is worth taking risks for, and other alterations in the brain help ensure that the lovelorn will do just that. Certain regions, scientists have found, are being deactivated, such as within the amygdala, associated with fear. "That's why you can do such insane things when you're in love," Fisher says. "You would never otherwise dream of driving across the country in 13 hours, but for love, you would."Sooner or later, excited brain messages reach the caudate nucleus, a dopamine-rich area where unconscious habits and skills, such as the ability to ride a bike, are stored.The attraction signal turns the love object into a habit, and then an obsession. According to a 1999 study in the journal Psychological Medicine, people newly in love have serotonin levels 40% lower than normal people do -- just like people with obsessive-compulsive disorders.Experiments in other mammals add to the human chemical findings. Female prairie voles, for example, develop a distinct preference for a specific male after mating, and the preference is associated with a 50% increase in dopamine in the nucleus accumbens.But when the monogamous vole is injected with a dopamine antagonist, blocking the activity of the chemical, she'll readily dump her partner for another.Using their headsKelly and Robert Iblings, now married for nine months, are fascinated by all this talk of nucleus accumbens, addiction and primitive mating instincts. Sure, they admit, they found each other attractive. But they were also making use of their front brains' sharp thinking skills. They were remembering painful past lessons and looking for signs of compatibility.They had each survived an earlier, failed engagement, and they knew what they were looking for this time around. They were listening to their front brains as they told them to look for compatibility, stability, shared values and commitment.From their first e-mail exchanges through eHarmony, an Internet dating service, the Iblings each felt they had found a unique mate. She liked to travel. So did he. They both love books and learning, have similar religious beliefs and come from loving, intact families. She no sooner sent an e-mail telling him about an exhibit she saw on a business trip to New York than he sent a message back telling her he knew of the exhibit because he had bought a book on it the day before.Coincidence, or soul mate?The front brain certainly gets involved as it ponders all of life's experiences and past mistakes, researchers say -- but not just the front brain. The nucleus accumbens, virtual swamp of dopamine that it is, is also holder of memories. Its quest for reward is influenced by childhood experiences, friends, previous failed engagements or the jerk who cheated on you. The sum of those experiences make some people attracted to a prince or a frog, a princess or a shrew.And, as it happens, practical matters such as whether a couple both like piña coladas and getting caught in the rain do matter in igniting passionate love.A research project headed by eHarmony Labs' Gonzaga interviewed 1,200 dating and newlywed couples. The results, reported in the July issue of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, found that those who reported similar interests and feelings were more satisfied. "Those who reported chemistry said they felt at ease, relaxed, connected. They knew they had some things in common," he says. "Chemistry is more than just being hot or handsome."Clearly, in the matters of love, the stars were aligned for the Iblings. When they met, they were ready for each other. But they were also attracted to each other. The chemistry was there. Most relationship researchers think it has to be.They had what it took to kick-start the relationship with an undeniable urgency, allowing two people to give up the candy store of other choices and commit to each other.Odds are that in two to four years, this urgency will fade -- and the couple will, if all goes well, settle in for the long haul with companionate love. Such peoples' lives are entwined, as are their property and bank accounts, and they begin to answer questionnaires differently. The rush and the urgency is gone, but they feel committed, emotionally close and stable.It is the state that many desire, yet it is the least studied. There's a reason for that. Most studies of couples are of college students and young newlyweds.Brown, however, has recently recruited volunteers for a study of people 40 to 65 who have been together for many years. She'll put them in fMRIs to see where love resides after the urgency fades. "It's unknown, the extent to which these original brain motivations are still active," she says. "Or whether companionate love has turned more cortical, more conscious thinking, more evaluative." Her first volunteers had their brains scanned this month.The free fall of love's first rush can happen at any age, whether people are 20 or 70, says Elaine Hatfield, psychology professor at the University of Hawaii and relationship researcher.What differs is that the older people get, the more memories they harbor of joy and trust, rejection and disappointment. And as people learn from experience, the front brain, with its logic and reason, probably gets a greater say."When you are young, passion and hope are so strong that's it's almost impossible to stop loving someone," Hatfield says. "After you've been kicked around by life, however, you start to have a dual response to handsome con men: 'Wow!' and 'Arrrrrrgh!'"It takes not will power but painful experience to make us wise."Somehow, it all comes together, for better or for worse, the sum total of what's found in the mating dance of the ancient reptilian brain, the passion of the limbic brain and the logic of the neocortex.Oh, what a ride
Wednesday
Searchable database of practice shown to be effective in preventing substance abuse or antecedents (http://casat.unr.edu/bestpractices/search.php)
Use the check boxes to indicate on which variable(s) you would like to conduct a search of evidence based drug prevention interventions. When you click the button near the bottom of the page ("Find Matching Practices"), an 'OR' search will be completed. Select as many attributes that interest you. The results will be ranked based on how many programs have matching attributes to the criteria you specify.
Other useful SAMHSA prevention sites:
http://captus.samhsa.gov/
http://prevtech.samhsa.gov/
Tuesday
"Stoners in the Mist"
New from Join Together (http://www.jointogether.org)
House Committee Approves Addiction/Mental Health Parity Bill In a historic move, the House Education and Labor Committee voted 33-9 to approve a strong mental-health and addiction parity bill, setting the stage for a possible House floor vote on the measure this fall.
U.S. Mayors Declare Drug War a Failure The mayors of America's cities have unanimously approved a resolution stating that the drug war 'has failed' and calling for a harm-reduction oriented approach to drug policy that focuses on public health. The U.S. Conference of Mayors adopted the resolution during its June 21-26 annual meeting in Los Angeles, calling for a "new bottom line" in drug policy that "concentrates more fully on reducing the negative consequences associated with drug abuse, while ensuring that our policies do not exacerbate these problems or create new social problems of their own; establishes quantifiable, short- and long-term objectives for drug policy; saves taxpayers money; and holds state and federal agencies responsible."
Wednesday
Wonder Drug Inspires Deep, Unwavering Love Of Pharmaceutical Companies
Wonder Drug Inspires Deep, Unwavering Love Of Pharmaceutical Companies
March 6, 2006 | Issue 42.10
NEW YORK-The Food and Drug Administration today approved the sale of the
drug PharmAmorin, a prescription tablet developed by Pfizer to treat
chronic
distrust of large prescription-drug manufacturers.
Pfizer executives characterized the FDA's approval as a "godsend" for
sufferers of independent-thinking-related mental-health disorders.
Enlarge Image
PharmAmorin, now relieving distrust of large pharmaceutical
conglomerates in
pharmacies nationwide.
"Many individuals today lack the deep, abiding affection for drug makers
that is found in healthy people, such as myself," Pfizer CEO Hank
McKinnell
said. "These tragic disorders are reaching epidemic levels, and as a
company
dedicated to promoting the health, well-being, and long life of our
company's public image, it was imperative that we did something to
combat
them."
Although many psychotropic drugs impart a generalized feeling of
well-being,
PharmAmorin is the first to induce and focus intense feelings of
affection
externally, toward for-profit drug makers. Pfizer representatives say
that,
if taken regularly, PharmAmorin can increase affection for and trust in
its
developers by as much as 96.5 percent.
"Out of a test group of 180, 172 study participants reported a dramatic
rise
in their passion for pharmaceutical companies," said Pfizer director of
clinical research Suzanne Frost. "And 167 asked their doctors about a
variety of prescription medications they had seen on TV."
Frost said a small percentage of test subjects showed an interest in
becoming lobbyists for one of the top five pharmaceutical companies, and
several browsed eBay for drug-company apparel.
PharmAmorin, available in 100-, 200-, and 400-mg tablets, is classified
as a
critical-thinking inhibitor, a family of drugs that holds great promise
for
the estimated 20 million Americans who suffer from Free-Thinking
Disorder.
Pfizer will also promote PharmAmorin in an aggressive, $34.6 million
and televised ad campaign.
One TV ad, set to debut during next Sunday's 60 Minutes telecast, shows
a
woman relaxing in her living room and reading a newspaper headlined
"Newest
Drug Company Scandal Undermines Public Trust." The camera zooms into the
tangled neural matter of her brain, revealing a sticky black substance
and a
purplish gas.
The narrator says, "She may show no symptoms, but in her brain,
irrational
fear and dislike of global pharmaceutical manufacturers is overwhelming
her
very peace of mind."
After a brief summary of PharmAmorin's benefits, the commercial
concludes
with the woman flying a kite across a sunny green meadow, the Pfizer
headquarters gleaming in the background.
PharmAmorin is the first drug of its kind, but Pfizer will soon face
competition from rival pharmaceutical giant Bristol-Myers Squibb. The
company is developing its own pro-pharmaceutical-company medication,
Brismysquibicin, which will induce warm feelings not just for drug
corporations in general, but solely for Bristol-Myers Squibb.
"A PharmAmorin user could find himself gravitating toward the products
of a
GlaxoSmithKline or Eli Lilly," BMS spokesman Andrew Fike said. "This
could
seriously impede the patient's prescription-drug-market acceptance, or
worse, Pfizer's profits in the long run."
"Brismysquibicin will be cheaper to produce and therefore far more
affordable to those on fixed incomes," Fike added.
The news of an affordable skepticism-inhibitor was welcomed by New York
physician Christine Blake-Mann, who runs a free clinic in Spanish
Harlem.
"A lot of my patients are very leery of the medical establishment,"
Blake-Mann said. "This will help them feel better about it, and save
money
at the same time."
PharmAmorin's side effects include nausea, upset stomach, and ignoring
the
side effects of prescription drug medication.
Tuesday
Slides from Session 2
The answer is that they're in the same pdf handout file with the slides from the first session. I won't be finished w/them until this week:
OVERVIEW OF SUBSTANCE ABUSE IN PUBLIC HEALTH
Friday
GW Substance Abuse Class, So Far
This is my first foray into the interactive world of blogging; so I hope that some of this year's students do give me some comments or responses here. I'm very interested in whether the blog can be used to fscilitate an electronic class discussion between class sessions.
Thursday
DSM-V Research Agenda: Substance Abuse/Psychosis Comorbidity
http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/33/4/947
Bruce J. Rounsaville1,2 2 VA CT Healthcare System, 950 Campbell Avenue (151D), West Haven, CT 06516
For diagnosis of patients with comorbid psychotic symptoms and substance use disorders (SUDs), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, makes clear distinctions between independent psychotic disorders (eg, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia) and substance-induced syndromes (eg, delirium, dementias). Most substance-induced psychotic symptoms are considered to be short lived and to resolve with sustained abstinence along with other symptoms of substance intoxication and withdrawal. These guidelines are challenged by practical difficulties in distinguishing between substance-induced and independent psychoses and by mounting evidence that marijuana use may be a contributing cause of schizophrenia. To inform the diagnostic distinction between substance-induced vs independent psychotic symptoms, 2 kinds of information could be sought from longitudinal research: (a) identification of early markers that clearly differentiate the 2 conditions and (b) more precise information about duration of psychotic symptoms induced by different substances. Evidence of this type could emerge from reanalysis of existing data from large-scale longitudinal studies of community samples. To inform possible nosological changes related to the possible schizophrenia-inducing role of marijuana (eg, designating a "cannabis-induced" subtype), a wide range of research evidence will be needed to clarify the relationship between effects of cannabis and schizophrenia symptoms. Ultimately, the ideal psychiatric nomenclature will define syndromes on the basis of established etiology and/or pathophysiology. Given the strong association between SUDs and psychotic disorders, research on the neurobiology of psychotic disorders could fruitfully include subjects with comorbid SUDs to shed light on shared etiology and pathophysiology.
Schizophrenia Bulletin 2007 33(4):947-952; doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm054;
Schizophrenia Bulletin Advance Access originally published online on June 7, 2007
1 To whom correspondence should be addressed; tel: 203-937-3486, fax: 203-937-3869, e-mail: bruce.rounsavill@yale.edu
Keywords: DSM-V / psychosis / substance use disorders / psychiatric diagnosis / psychiatric epidemiology / alcohol
Is Cocaine Desire Reduced by N-Acetylcysteine?
OBJECTIVE: Animal models suggest that N-acetylcysteine inhibits cocaine-seeking. The present pilot study evaluated whether N-acetylcysteine would suppress reactivity to cocaine-related cues in cocaine-dependent humans. METHOD: In this double-blind, placebo-controlled trial, 15 participants received N-acetylcysteine or placebo during a 3-day hospitalization. Participants were crossed over to receive the opposite condition on a second, identical 3-day stay occurring 4 days later. During each hospital stay, participants completed a cue-reactivity procedure that involved collecting psychophysical and subjective data in response to slides depicting cocaine and cocaine use. RESULTS: While taking N-acetylcysteine, participants reported less desire to use and less interest in response to cocaine slides and watched cocaine slides for less time. CONCLUSIONS: The inhibition of cocaine cue reactivity is consistent with existing preclinical data and supports the use of N-acetylcysteine as a treatment for cocaine dependence.
Am J Psychiatry 164:1115-1117, July 2007doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.7.1115
Steven D. LaRowe, Ph.D., Hugh Myrick, M.D., Sarra Hedden, M.S., Pascale Mardikian, M.D., Michael Saladin, Ph.D., Aimee McRae, Pharm.D., Kathleen Brady, M.D., Ph.D., Peter W. Kalivas, Ph.D. and Robert Malcolm, M.D.
Tuesday
Symptoms of Tobacco Dependence After Brief Intermittent Use -- Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine
http://www.mdlinx.com/PsychLinx/newsl-article.cfm/1905225
DiFranza, J.R., et al. - To extend the findings of the first Development and Assessment of Nicotine Dependence in Youth study by using diagnostic criteria for tobacco dependence and a biochemical measure of nicotine intake...Conclusion: The most susceptible youths lose autonomy over tobacco within a day or 2 of first inhaling from a cigarette. The appearance of tobacco withdrawal symptoms and failed attempts at cessation can precede daily smoking; ICD-10–defined dependence can precede daily smoking and typically appears before consumption reaches 2 cigarettes per day...
The Development and Assessment of Nicotine Dependence in Youth–2 Study
Joseph R. DiFranza, MD; Judith A. Savageau, MPH; Kenneth Fletcher, PhD; Jennifer O’Loughlin, PhD; Lori Pbert, PhD; Judith K. Ockene, PhD; Ann D. McNeill, PhD; Jennifer Hazelton, BA; Karen Friedman, BA; Gretchen Dussault, BA; Connie Wood, MSW; Robert J. Wellman, PhD
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161:704-710.
Objective:To extend the findings of the first Development and Assessment of Nicotine Dependence in Youth study by using diagnostic criteria for tobacco dependence and a biochemical measure of nicotine intake. The first study found that symptoms of dependence commonly appeared soon after the onset of intermittent smoking.
Design:A 4-year prospective study.
Setting: Public schools in 6 Massachusetts communities.
Participants: A cohort of 1246 sixth-grade students.
Interventions: Eleven interviews.
Main Outcome Measures: Loss of autonomy over tobacco as measured by the Hooked on Nicotine Checklist, and tobacco dependence as defined in International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision (ICD-10).
Results: Among the 217 inhalers, 127 lost autonomy over their tobacco use, 10% having done so within 2 days and 25% having done so within 30 days of first inhaling from a cigarette; half had lost autonomy by the time they were smoking 7 cigarettes per month. Among the 83 inhalers who developed ICD-10–defined dependence, half had done so by the time they were smoking 46 cigarettes per month. At the interview following the onset of ICD-10–defined dependence, the median salivary cotinine concentration of current smokers was 5.35 ng/mL, a level that falls well below the cutoff used to distinguish active from passive smokers.
Conclusions: The most susceptible youths lose autonomy over tobacco within a day or 2 of first inhaling from a cigarette. The appearance of tobacco withdrawal symptoms and failed attempts at cessation can precede daily smoking; ICD-10–defined dependence can precede daily smoking and typically appears before consumption reaches 2 cigarettes per day.
Monday
NIAAA Identifies Five Subtypes of Alcohol Dependence
Researchers Identify Alcoholism Subtypes
Analyses of a national sample of individuals with alcohol dependence (alcoholism) reveal five distinct subtypes of the disease, according to a new study by scientists at the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA), part of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
"Our findings should help dispel the popular notion of the ‘typical alcoholic,’” notes first author Howard B. Moss, M.D., NIAAA Associate Director for Clinical and Translational Research. “We find that young adults comprise the largest group of alcoholics in this country, and nearly 20 percent of alcoholics are highly functional and well-educated with good incomes. More than half of the alcoholics in the United States have no multigenerational family history of the disease, suggesting that their form of alcoholism was unlikely to have genetic causes.”
“Clinicians have long recognized diverse manifestations of alcoholism,” adds NIAAA Director Ting-Kai Li, M.D, “and researchers have tried to understand why some alcoholics improve with specific medications and psychotherapies while others do not. The classification system described in this study will have broad application in both clinical and research settings.” A report of the study is now available online in the journal Drug and Alcohol Dependence.
Previous efforts to identify alcoholism subtypes focused primarily on individuals who were hospitalized or otherwise receiving treatment for their alcoholism. However, recent reports from NIAAA’s National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC), a nationally representative epidemiological study of alcohol, drug, and mental disorders in the United States, suggest that only about one-fourth of individuals with alcoholism have ever received treatment. Thus, a substantial proportion of people with alcoholism were not represented in the samples previously used to define subtypes of this disease.
In the current study, Dr. Moss and colleagues applied advanced statistical methods to data from the NESARC. Their analyses focused on the 1,484 NESARC survey respondents who met diagnostic criteria for alcohol dependence, and included individuals in treatment as well as those not seeking treatment. The researchers identified unique subtypes of alcoholism based on respondents’ family history of alcoholism, age of onset of regular drinking and alcohol problems, symptom patterns of alcohol dependence and abuse, and the presence of additional substance abuse and mental disorders:
Young Adult subtype: 31.5 percent of U.S. alcoholics. Young adult drinkers, with relatively low rates of co-occurring substance abuse and other mental disorders, a low rate of family alcoholism, and who rarely seek any kind of help for their drinking.
Young Antisocial subtype: 21 percent of U.S. alcoholics. Tend to be in their mid-twenties, had early onset of regular drinking, and alcohol problems. More than half come from families with alcoholism, and about half have a psychiatric diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder. Many have major depression, bipolar disorder, and anxiety problems. More than 75 percent smoked cigarettes and marijuana, and many also had cocaine and opiate addictions. More than one-third of these alcoholics seek help for their drinking.
Functional subtype: 19.5 percent of U.S. alcoholics. Typically middle-aged, well-educated, with stable jobs and families. About one-third have a multigenerational family history of alcoholism, about one-quarter had major depressive illness sometime in their lives, and nearly 50 percent were smokers.
Intermediate Familial subtype: 19 percent of U.S. alcoholics. Middle-aged, with about 50 percent from families with multigenerational alcoholism. Almost half have had clinical depression, and 20 percent have had bipolar disorder. Most of these individuals smoked cigarettes, and nearly one in five had problems with cocaine and marijuana use. Only 25 percent ever sought treatment for their problem drinking.
Chronic Severe subtype: 9 percent of U.S. alcoholics. Comprised mostly of middle-aged individuals who had early onset of drinking and alcohol problems, with high rates of Antisocial Personality Disorder and criminality. Almost 80 percent come from families with multigenerational alcoholism. They have the highest rates of other psychiatric disorders including depression, bipolar disorder, and anxiety disorders as well as high rates of smoking, and marijuana, cocaine, and opiate dependence. Two-thirds of these alcoholics seek help for their drinking problems, making them the most prevalent type of alcoholic in treatment.
The NIAAA subtypes report is published online in the journal Drug and Alcohol Dependence.
Reference:Moss, HB, Chen, CM, Yi, HY. (2007) Subtypes of alcohol dependence in a nationally representative sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, Article in Press, Corrected Proof; available online June 26, 2007; doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.016.
Other Items From Join together:
Health Plans Use Internet to Give Information, Not Counseling
12-Step Treatment More Effective than Alternative, Study Says
National Conference on Pain, Opioids, and Addiction
Study Shows Most Treatment Effective Against Alcoholism
Addiction: Why Can't They Just Stop?
National Directory of Drug and Alcohol Abuse Treatment Programs 2006
Gaming junkies get no diagnosis: AMA report on video game "Addiction"
http://www.latimes.com/business/la-fi-addict28jun28,0,6082192.story?coll=la-home-center
Gaming junkies get no diagnosis
By Alex PhamLA Times Staff WriterJune 28, 2007
Video-game buffs might feel hooked on their favorite titles, but theywon't be officially addicted anytime soon.Saying the issue needed more study, the American Medical Assn. onWednesday scaled back a controversial proposal that sought to declareexcessive video-game playing a mental disorder akin to pathologicalgambling.The association also decided against urging parents to limit to twohours a day the amount of time their kids play video games, watchtelevision and surf the Internet."While more study is needed on the addictive potential of video games,the AMA remains concerned about the behavioral, health and societaleffects of video-game and Internet overuse," Dr. Ronald M. Davis, theassociation's president, said in a statement from its annual meeting inChicago. "We urge parents to closely monitor their children's use ofvideo games and the Internet."The 250,000-member physician organization drew national headlines lastweek by pressing forward on a proposal to "strongly encourage" thatvideo-game addiction be labeled a formal disorder. The proposal wouldhave asked the American Psychiatric Assn. to consider including"video-game addiction as a formal diagnostic disorder" in the Diagnosticand Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, considered by experts to bethe authoritative handbook on mental illness.Instead, the medical association Wednesday removed the word "addiction"and decided to simply forward its report expressing concerns about"video-game overuse" to the psychiatric group, which is revising itsmental-health manual.Maressa Hecht Orzack, director of the computer-addiction studies centerat McLean Hospital in Belmont, Mass., said the word choice wasirrelevant."The fact is, it's a behavior that's out of control," Orzack said,noting that some of her patients have trouble with school, work andtheir relationships because of their game-playing habits. "Whether youcall it addiction, overuse or excessive use, it's the same thing. It's acondition that interferes with a person's mental health."But some in the video-game industry, including the EntertainmentSoftware Assn., were pleased with the toned-down language. The tradegroup for the $30-billion game industry "supports mental-health experts,the APA and others within the AMA who agree that it would be prematureto conclude that video-game 'addiction' is a mental disorder," saidMichael Gallagher, its president.Industry executives were less happy with another recommendation in thereport approved Wednesday: The physicians' organization plans to lobbythe Federal Trade Commission to improve the current voluntary video-gamerating system, which is now run by the industry-funded EntertainmentSoftware Rating Board."We would like to see a ratings system that better alerts parents to thecontent of the video game and recommended age of the player, so they candecide whether or not their child should be playing it," the AMA's Davissaid.The board defended its system, which assigns ratings based on the levelof violence or sexual innuendo in games.The medical group's proposal to review the ratings system "seems todisregard the fact that the vast majority of parents are satisfied withthe ESRB ratings and use them regularly to choose games for theirchildren," ratings board President Patricia Vance said in a statement.
Gene Variant Increases Risk for Alcoholism Following Childhood Abuse
Wednesday
FDA Announces Final Rule Establishing Current Good ManufacturingPractices forDietary Supplements
The Food and Drug Administration (FDA) today announced its final ruleestablishing current good manufacturing practices (cGMPs) for dietarysupplements.In the Dietary Supplement Health and Education Act of 1994 (DSHEA),Congressgave the Secretary of Health and Human Services and, by delegation, FDAauthority to issue regulations establishing cGMPs for dietarysupplements. The GMPs will require that dietary supplements are produced in a qualitymanner,are not adulterated with contaminants or impurities, and are accuratelylabeledto reflect the ingredients in the product. The cGMPs apply to alldomestic andforeign companies that manufacture, package, or hold dietary supplementsintended for sale in U.S. commerce, including those involved with theactivitiesof testing, quality control, packaging, labeling, and distributing.In a companion document, FDA is also announcing today an interim finalrule(IFR) that outlines a petition process for manufacturers to request anexemptionto the cGMP requirement for 100 percent identity testing of specificdietaryingredients. If the manufacturer can provide sufficient documentationthat thesupplier maintains appropriate in-process manufacturing controls and hasconsistently produced the dietary ingredient over a period of time, themanufacturer may be exempted from the testing requirement. FDA issolicitingcomment from the public on the IFR. There will be a 90-day commentperiod.Written comments may be submitted to the Dockets Management Branch(HFA-305),Food and Drug Administration, 5630 Fishers Lane, Room 1061, Rockville,MD 20852or on line at:http://www.fda.gov/dockets/ecommentsAdditional information regarding the cGMPs is available through theCFSANwebsite athttp://www.cfsan.fda.gov/~dms/supplmnt.html.NOTE:* TO GET PAST ISSUES OF FDA-DSFL, go to Electronic InformationNetworks:http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/~dms/infonet.html.********************************FDA Web Page Addresses:* Dietary Supplements http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/~dms/supplmnt.html* Food Labeling and Nutrition http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/label.html* Infant Formula http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/~dms/inf-toc.html* Qualified Health Claims http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/~dms/lab-qhc.html* Center for Food Safety and Applied Nutrition http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/list.htmlOther Federal Government Links:* Federal Trade Commission http://www.ftc.gov/* Office of Dietary Supplements, NIH http://dietary-supplements.info.nih.gov/* US Department of Agriculture - Food Safety and Inspection Service http://www.fsis.usda.gov/ - Food and Nutrition Service http://www.fns.usda.gov/fns/Non-Federal Government Links:* National Academy of Sciences http://www.iom.edu/CMS/3788.aspx